The present research explored the relationship between alexithymia, self-reported depression and trait anxiety and dreaming experience in a non-clinical sample. Characteristics of the alexithymia trait, such as the deficit in the symbolic representation of emotions and the limited imaginative ability, can have an impact on the richness and quality of dream experience. Depression and trait anxiety are both reported to be related to alexithymia and dreaming. A group of 30 non-clinical subjects (20 females) participated in the study. Alexithymia was assessed with the Toronto Structured Interview for Alexithymia (TSIA), an observer-report instrument composed of 24 questions that assesses a general subjective level of alexithymia and 4 dimensions of alexithymia: Identifying emotional feelings (DIF), Describing emotional feelings (DDF), Imaginal Processes (IMP); and Externally oriented thinking (EOT). The subjective dreaming experience was assessed with the Mannheim Dream Questionnaire (MADRE), an instrument that was designed to elicit some form of dream history including dream recall frequency, use and correlates of dream, attitude towards dream and presence of different kinds of dreams experience, such as nightmares, lucid dreams, and déjà-vu. The BDI-2 and the STAI-Y2 were administered to evaluate negative affect. Two multivariate multiple regression analysis were carried on. Results showed that alexithymia was related to dream correlates and to attitude towards dream. Trait anxiety was found to be related to nightmares frequency and self-reported depression to déjà-vu event. In the future, it would be interesting to use a larger and more representative sample. The use of clinical population, such as nightmare sufferers or patients with sleep and/or mental disorders, could possibly disclosure further meaningful relationships among affect dysregulation, affect modulation and self-reported retrospective dream characteristics.
Published in | American Journal of Applied Psychology (Volume 8, Issue 6) |
DOI | 10.11648/j.ajap.20190806.13 |
Page(s) | 121-132 |
Creative Commons |
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is properly cited. |
Copyright |
Copyright © The Author(s), 2019. Published by Science Publishing Group |
Alexithymia, Depression, Trait Anxiety, Dreams Correlates, Attitude Towards Dreams
[1] | Taylor G. J., Bagby, M. R., and Parker, J. D. A., “Disorders of Affect Regulation: Alexithymia in Medical and Psychiatric Illness”, 1999, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. |
[2] | Caretti, V., La Barbera, D., and Capraro, G., “La Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) ”, in Alessitimia, valutazione e trattamento, Vincenzo Caretti, V., and La Barbera, D., Casa Editrice Astrolabio - Ubaldini Editore, Roma, 2005, pp. 17-23. |
[3] | Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M., and Parker, J. D. A., “I disturbi della regolazione affettiva”, in L’alessitimia nelle malattie mediche e psichiatriche (Tr. it., 2000), 2007, Roma: Fioriti. |
[4] | Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M, and Parker, J. D. A. (2016). What’s in the name “alexithymia”? A commentary on “Affective agnosia: Expansion of the alexithymia construct and a new opportunity to integrate and extend Freud’s legacy. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 68, 1006-1020. doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.05.025. |
[5] | Apfel, R. J., and Sifneos, P. E. (1979). Alexithymia: Concept and measurement. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 32, 180–190. |
[6] | Krystal, H. (1979). Alexithymia and psychotherapy. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 33, 17–31. |
[7] | De Gennaro, L., Ferrara, M., Cristiani, R., Curcio, G., Martiradonna, V., and Bertini, M., (2003). Alexithymia and dream recall upon spontaneous morning awakening. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 301-306. |
[8] | Warnes, H. (1986). Alexithymia, clinical and therapeutic aspects. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 46, 96–104. |
[9] | Lumley, M. A., and Bazydlo, R. A. (2000). The relationship of alexithymia characteristics to dreaming. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 48, 561–567. |
[10] | Parker, J. D. A., Bauermann, T. M., and Smith, C. T. (2000). Alexithymia and impoverished dream content: Evidence from rapid eye movement sleep awakenings. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 486–491. |
[11] | Parker, J. D., Taylor, G. J., and Bagby, R. M. (2003). The 20-Item Toronto Alexithymia Scale. III. Reliability and factorial validity in a community population. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 55, 269–275. |
[12] | Nielsen, T. A., Taylor, G. J., and Bagby, J. (2011). Dreaming correlates of alexithymia among sleep-disordered patients. Dreaming, 1, 1-16. |
[13] | Tantam, D., Kalucy, R., and Brown, D. G. (1982). Sleep, scratching and dreams in eczema: A new approach to alexithymia. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 37, 26–35. |
[14] | Monday, J., Montplaisir, J., and Malo, J. L. (1987). Dream process in asthmatic subjects with nocturnal attacks. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 638–640. |
[15] | Nielsen, T., and Levin, R. (2007). Nightmares, Bad Dreams, and Emotion Dysregulation: A Review and New Neurocognitive Model of Dreaming. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18 (2), 84-88. |
[16] | Quellet, L., Nielsen, T. A., Montplaisir, J., Cartier, A., Malo, J. L., and Lassonde, M. (1996). L’alexithymie, re´ponse affective et reˆves: Investigation en laboratoire de trois caracte´ristiques sous-jacentesaude´ficitdel’expressiondese´motions. Revue Internationale De Psychopathologie, 23, 491–503. |
[17] | Bazydlo, R., Lumley, M. A., and Roehrs, T. (2001). Alexithymia and polysomnographic measures of sleep in healthy adults. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 56-61. |
[18] | Taylor, G. J., Parker, J. D. A., Bagby, R. M., and Bourke, M. P. (1996). Relationships between alexithymia and psychological characteristics associated with eating disorders. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 41, 561-568. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999 (96) 00224-3. |
[19] | Beauchemin K. M., and Hays, P. J. (1996). Dreaming away depression: the role of REM sleep and dreaming in affective disorders. Affective Disorders, 41, 125-1. |
[20] | Sandman, N., Valli, K., Kronholm, E., Revonsuo, A., Laatikainen T., and Tiina Paunio, T. (2015). Nightmares: Risk Factors Among the Finnish General Adult Population. Sleep, 1, 38 (4), 507-14. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4560. |
[21] | Levin, R., and Nielsen, T. A. (2007). Disturbed dreaming, posttraumatic stress disorder, and affect distress: A review and neurocognitive model. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 482–528. |
[22] | Levin, R., and Hurvich, M. S. (1995). Nightmares and annihilation anxiety. Psychoanalytic Psychology 12 (2), 247-258. doi.org/10.1037/h0079625. |
[23] | Spoormaker, V. I., Schredl, M., and Van den Bout, J. (2006). Nightmares: from anxiety symptom to sleep disorder. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 10, 19–31. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2005.06.001. |
[24] | Bagby, R. M., Taylor, G. J., Parker, J. D. A., and Dickens, S. E., (2006). The development of the Toronto Structured Interview for Alexithymia: Item selection, factor structure, reliability and concurrent validity. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 75, 25-39. doi.org/10.1159/000089224. |
[25] | Caretti, V., Porcelli, P., Solano, L., Schimmenti, A., Bagby, R. M., and Taylor, G. J. (2011). Reliability and validity of the Toronto Structured Interview for Alexithymia in a mixed clinical and nonclinical sample from Italy. Psychiatry Research, 187 (3), 432-436. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.02.015. |
[26] | Inslegers, R., Löbel, S., Dittrich, D., Bagby, R. M., Taylor, G. J., Quilty, L. C., Spitzer, C., Barnow, S., Mathier, F., Jenewein, J., Freyberger, H. J., Rufer, M. (2013). The Dutch language version of the Toronto Structured Interview for Alexithymia: reliability, factor structure and concurrent validity. Psychologica Belgica, 53 (1), 93-116. doi.org/10.5934/pb-53-1-93. |
[27] | Montebarocci, O., and Surcinelli, P. (2018). Correlations between TSIA and TAS-20 and their relation to self-reported negative affect: a study using a multi-method approach in the assessment of alexithymia in a nonclinical sample from Italy. Psychiatry Research, 270, 187-193. doi: org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.09.036. |
[28] | Taylor, G. J., and Bagby, R. M. 2013. Psychoanalysis and empirical research: the example of alexithymia. Journal of the American Psychiatric Association 61: 99–133. doi. 10.1177/0003065112474066. |
[29] | Tibon, S., Weinberger, Y., Handelzaltz, J. E., and Porcelli, P. (2005). Construct validation of the Rorschach reality-fantasy scale in alexithymia. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 22, 508-523. doi.org/10.1037/07369735.22.4.508. |
[30] | Sekely, A., Taylor, G. J., and Bagby, R. M., (2018). Developing a short version of the Toronto Structured Interview for Alexithymia using item response theory. Psychiatry Research, 266, 218-227. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.03.002. |
[31] | Schredl, M. (2002). Questionnaires and diaries as research instruments in dream research: methodological issues. Dreaming, 12, 17–26. |
[32] | Schredl, M., Berres, S., Klingauf, A., Schellhaas, S., and Göritz, A. (2014). The Mannheim Dream questionnaire (MADRE): Retest reliability, age and gender effects. International Journal of Dream Research, 7 (2). 141-147. doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2014.2.16675. |
[33] | Pedrabissi, L., and Santinello, M., Verifica della validità dello STAI forma Y di Spielberger [Verification of the validity of the STAI, Form Y, by Spielberger], (1989), Giunti Organizzazioni Speciali. |
[34] | Spielberger, C. D., “State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: Bibliography (2nd ed.) ”, 1989, Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. |
[35] | Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., and Brown, G. K., “Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory–II”, 1996, San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. |
[36] | Ghisi, M., Flebus, G. B., Montano, A., Sanavio, E., and Sica, C., “Beck Depression Inventory – II (BDI-II) Manuale”, 2006, Firenze: O. S. Organizzazioni Speciali. |
[37] | Godin, I., Montplaisir, J., Gagnon, J. F., and Nielsen, T. (2013). Alexithymia associated with nightmare distress in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Sleep, 36 (12), 1957-1962. |
[38] | American Academy of Sleep Medicine, “The international classification of sleep disorders. (ICSD-3) ”, 2014, Darien, IL: AASM. |
[39] | Schredl, M., and Erlacher, D. 2004. Lucid dreaming frequency and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1463-1473. |
[40] | Funkhouser, A. T. 2009. Survey data, http://www.deja-experience-research.org. |
[41] | Neppe, V. M., “The Psychology of Déjà Vu: Have I Been Here Before?”, 1983, Witwatersrand University Press. |
[42] | Clark, L. A., and Watson D. (1995). Constructing validity: basic issues in objective scale development. Psychological Assessment, 7, 309–319. |
[43] | Netemeyer, R. G., Bearden, W. O., and Sharma, S., “Scaling Procedures: Issues and Applications”, 2004, Thousand Oaks, Sage Publications. |
[44] | Cortina, J. M. (1993). What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 98–104. |
[45] | Fleiss, J. L., and Shrout P. E. (1977). The effects of measurement errors on some multivariate procedures. American Journal of Public Health 67, 1188–1191. |
[46] | Landis J. R., and Koch G. G. 1977. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 33, 159–174. |
[47] | Bernstein, D. M., and Belicki, K. (1995). On the psychometric properties of retrospective dream content questionnaires. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 15, 351-364. |
[48] | Zadra, A., Pilon, M., and Donderi, D. C. (2006). Variety and intensity of emotions in bad dreams and nightmares. Journal of nervous and mental disease, 194, 249-254. Canadian Psychology, 34 (2a), 294. |
[49] | Sno, H. N., Linszen, D. H., and De Jonghe, F. (1992b). Empirical research on déjà vu experiences: a review. Behavioral Neurology, 5, 151-161. |
[50] | Sno, H. N., and Draaisma, D. (1993). An early Dutch study of déjà vu experiences. Psychological Medicine, 23 (1): 17-26, 1993, Doi: 10.1017/S0033291700038812}. |
[51] | Wolfradt, U. (2000). Zeitschrift für klinische Psychologie, Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 48 (4): 359-376. |
[52] | Arango-Muñoz, S. (2013b). Scaffolded memory and metacognitive feelings. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 4 (1): 135-152. |
[53] | Lacinova, L., Michalcakova, R. N., Sirucek, J., Jezek, S., Chromec, J., Masopustova, Z., Urbanek, T., Brazdil, M. (2016). Deja Vu Experiences in Healthy Czech Adults. Journal of Nervous and Mental disease, 204 (12), 925–930. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000573. |
[54] | Funkhouser, A., and Schredl, M. (2010). The frequency of déjà vu (déjà rêve) and the effects of age, dream recall frequency and personality factors. International Journal of Dream Research 3/1, 60-64. |
[55] | Zunker, M., Althoff, H. K., Apel, J., Lässig, H. S., Schültke, L., and Schredl, M. (2015). Comparing questionnaire and diary measures for eliciting nightmare frequency. International Journal of Dream Research, 8/2, 129-134. |
APA Style
Ornella Montebarocci, Sara Giovagnoli. (2019). Alexithymia, Depression, Trait-anxiety and Their Relation to Self-reported Retrospective Dream Experience. American Journal of Applied Psychology, 8(6), 121-132. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajap.20190806.13
ACS Style
Ornella Montebarocci; Sara Giovagnoli. Alexithymia, Depression, Trait-anxiety and Their Relation to Self-reported Retrospective Dream Experience. Am. J. Appl. Psychol. 2019, 8(6), 121-132. doi: 10.11648/j.ajap.20190806.13
AMA Style
Ornella Montebarocci, Sara Giovagnoli. Alexithymia, Depression, Trait-anxiety and Their Relation to Self-reported Retrospective Dream Experience. Am J Appl Psychol. 2019;8(6):121-132. doi: 10.11648/j.ajap.20190806.13
@article{10.11648/j.ajap.20190806.13, author = {Ornella Montebarocci and Sara Giovagnoli}, title = {Alexithymia, Depression, Trait-anxiety and Their Relation to Self-reported Retrospective Dream Experience}, journal = {American Journal of Applied Psychology}, volume = {8}, number = {6}, pages = {121-132}, doi = {10.11648/j.ajap.20190806.13}, url = {https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajap.20190806.13}, eprint = {https://article.sciencepublishinggroup.com/pdf/10.11648.j.ajap.20190806.13}, abstract = {The present research explored the relationship between alexithymia, self-reported depression and trait anxiety and dreaming experience in a non-clinical sample. Characteristics of the alexithymia trait, such as the deficit in the symbolic representation of emotions and the limited imaginative ability, can have an impact on the richness and quality of dream experience. Depression and trait anxiety are both reported to be related to alexithymia and dreaming. A group of 30 non-clinical subjects (20 females) participated in the study. Alexithymia was assessed with the Toronto Structured Interview for Alexithymia (TSIA), an observer-report instrument composed of 24 questions that assesses a general subjective level of alexithymia and 4 dimensions of alexithymia: Identifying emotional feelings (DIF), Describing emotional feelings (DDF), Imaginal Processes (IMP); and Externally oriented thinking (EOT). The subjective dreaming experience was assessed with the Mannheim Dream Questionnaire (MADRE), an instrument that was designed to elicit some form of dream history including dream recall frequency, use and correlates of dream, attitude towards dream and presence of different kinds of dreams experience, such as nightmares, lucid dreams, and déjà-vu. The BDI-2 and the STAI-Y2 were administered to evaluate negative affect. Two multivariate multiple regression analysis were carried on. Results showed that alexithymia was related to dream correlates and to attitude towards dream. Trait anxiety was found to be related to nightmares frequency and self-reported depression to déjà-vu event. In the future, it would be interesting to use a larger and more representative sample. The use of clinical population, such as nightmare sufferers or patients with sleep and/or mental disorders, could possibly disclosure further meaningful relationships among affect dysregulation, affect modulation and self-reported retrospective dream characteristics.}, year = {2019} }
TY - JOUR T1 - Alexithymia, Depression, Trait-anxiety and Their Relation to Self-reported Retrospective Dream Experience AU - Ornella Montebarocci AU - Sara Giovagnoli Y1 - 2019/12/07 PY - 2019 N1 - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajap.20190806.13 DO - 10.11648/j.ajap.20190806.13 T2 - American Journal of Applied Psychology JF - American Journal of Applied Psychology JO - American Journal of Applied Psychology SP - 121 EP - 132 PB - Science Publishing Group SN - 2328-5672 UR - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajap.20190806.13 AB - The present research explored the relationship between alexithymia, self-reported depression and trait anxiety and dreaming experience in a non-clinical sample. Characteristics of the alexithymia trait, such as the deficit in the symbolic representation of emotions and the limited imaginative ability, can have an impact on the richness and quality of dream experience. Depression and trait anxiety are both reported to be related to alexithymia and dreaming. A group of 30 non-clinical subjects (20 females) participated in the study. Alexithymia was assessed with the Toronto Structured Interview for Alexithymia (TSIA), an observer-report instrument composed of 24 questions that assesses a general subjective level of alexithymia and 4 dimensions of alexithymia: Identifying emotional feelings (DIF), Describing emotional feelings (DDF), Imaginal Processes (IMP); and Externally oriented thinking (EOT). The subjective dreaming experience was assessed with the Mannheim Dream Questionnaire (MADRE), an instrument that was designed to elicit some form of dream history including dream recall frequency, use and correlates of dream, attitude towards dream and presence of different kinds of dreams experience, such as nightmares, lucid dreams, and déjà-vu. The BDI-2 and the STAI-Y2 were administered to evaluate negative affect. Two multivariate multiple regression analysis were carried on. Results showed that alexithymia was related to dream correlates and to attitude towards dream. Trait anxiety was found to be related to nightmares frequency and self-reported depression to déjà-vu event. In the future, it would be interesting to use a larger and more representative sample. The use of clinical population, such as nightmare sufferers or patients with sleep and/or mental disorders, could possibly disclosure further meaningful relationships among affect dysregulation, affect modulation and self-reported retrospective dream characteristics. VL - 8 IS - 6 ER -